Learning

Article curated by

From the moment we're born, we begin to learn about the world around us, and this continues for the rest of our lives. But exactly how does learning happen in the brain? And how does it change throughout out lives? With reference to other animals and how they seem to learn, scientists are working hard to find out the answers.

Learning Image credit: Public domain

Learning in the brain

When we learn, our brains change: some pathways become stronger and others weaker. This ability to change is known as plasticity, and the ease with which it happens varies throughout our lives. When we are young, our brains are more plastic to enable us to learn rapidly but, as we age, efficiency and stability become more important and our brains become less flexible. However, we don't know exactly what causes these changes, or why they happen at different times for different abilities. If it were possible to reverse changes, and put the adult brain back into learning mode, this could be hugely beneficial, not just for general learning (who hasn't wished they could pick up a language as easily as a child?), but also for anyone who has suffered a stroke or other form of brain damage[1].
Learn more about Rewiring the Brain.

 Lightbulb Institutes Delving Deeper
List of Institutes Researching Rewiring the Brain
Things We Don’t Know (2 researchers)
3

Openclipartvectors (CC0 Public Domain via Pixabay)
The hippocampus is vital for forming long term memories. Image credit: Public domain
We store both information and processes in memories in the brain. We know that the hippocampus is vital for converting short term memories into long term memories, which are then stored in the cortex (the surface layer of the brain). But how and where in the cortex memories are stored is unclear[2]. The best current theory is that they are stored in variety of systems distributed across the cortex. Theoretically, this should allow an almost infinite number of memories to be stored.

In order to enter long term storage, memories must be consolidated. This process begins within minutes of learning, but continues for up to a few weeks. Exactly what is happening in this process isn’t fully understood, but, for many years, scientists thought that the hippocampus repeatedly activates regions of the cortex, strengthening the connections there and effectively transferring the memory from the hippocampus to the cortex, where it can be stored permanently. However, a recent study in mice suggests that the hippocampal memory and the cortex version are created at the same time, but that the cortical memory isn't used for the first few days after it is formed – the memory is immature or silent. If connections between the hippocampus and cortex are blocked, the memory never matures. If the hippocampus is damaged, the ability to store long term memories can even be completely lost.
Learn more about Memory in the brain.

 Lightbulb Institutes Delving Deeper
List of Institutes Researching Memory in the brain
Things We Don’t Know (1 researchers)
2

Despite not knowing exactly how it works, it seems humans are getting better at learning, and more intelligent, although there may be more to this finding that meets the eye. IQ tests are designed to keep the population average at 100; but this means the tests have to get harder and harder – by about 1 point every 3 years. That may not sound much, but it would mean half of the world population from the time of WW1 would be classed as "mentally disabled" by modern standards! Clearly, something is wrong.

Various theories exist have attempted to explain why. Could it be education? On average, we stay in school longer, and maybe we are taught in a different way – less rote learning and more abstract thinking, which makes us better suited to the IQ tests than our parents and grandparents. Or are we simply better at sitting exams? Another idea is that better nutrition is responsible for the rise. Prenatal nutrition affects birth weight, and birth weight is correlated to higher IQs. One study also found that iodine defficiency in pregnant mothers is linked to lower IQ scores in their children.

James Flynn, who the effect is named after, doesn't think we are any more intelligent than our ancestors.It's like a weightlifter and swimmer. They may have the same muscles when they were fertilised in the womb, but they would have different muscles at autopsy, wouldn't they? he said, in an interview with the BBC So today at autopsy, certain portions of our brain, for example those which use logic and abstraction, would have been exercised more and look differently. Other portions of the brain would have shrivelled a bit.

 Lightbulb

Learning through the lifespan

Public Domain via TawnyNina (Pixabay)
Babies' brains are optimised for learning. Image credit: Public domain

From the moment they are born, babies start learning about the world around them. But how can we find out what they know? Most psychological studies involve instructions, questions and answers. But you can’t ask a newborn baby to press a button, answer a question, or rate something on a scale. One thing many newborn babies can do, and enjoy doing, is looking at things. So one common way of finding out what babies like is to give them two options and see which one they choose to look at. Using this technique, we now know that babies are born with a preference for faces. Even just a few hours after birth, they will choose to look at a face, or something that resembles a face, over almost any other stimuli.

There is still debate over whether babies are innately drawn specifically to faces, or whether they are drawn to various low-level properties that faces have, such as high contrast and top-heavy shape. Whatever it is, over our first few months of life, we become experts in recognising and categorising faces, and this practice continues throughout our lives.

 Lightbulb Institutes Delving Deeper
List of Institutes Researching innate face preference in infants
Things We Don’t Know (1 researchers)

These looking time experiments can be taken one step further. If you show a baby the same image over and over again, they will get bored and stop looking at it. If you then show them something new, they will look longer again. This can be used to work out what they can discriminate. If, for example, they get bored of a red square, but look longer at a red circle, you know they can tell squares are different from circles. If a green square continued to bore them, you would know they couldn’t distinguish red from green.

Some scientists believe this technique can be used to understand infants' conceptual knowledge. If, for example, they look longer at an impossible event, like an object teleporting, they are said to be surprised by that event, and therefore understand that it shouldn't happen. Research suggests babies understand physical concepts including: solidity (they are surprised when an object is able to pass through another object), occlusion (that objects don't disappear when they are blocked from view), and containment (a big object can't fit into a smaller one).

Of course, there are all sorts of concerns about the validity of this method, and it has led to some strange finding, with babies supposedly understanding concepts that children don’t seem to grasp until much later when you ask them directly. For example, babies won't reach for a hidden object until 8 months, but looking time experiments suggest they know it's there. So why don't they reach? More research is definitely needed.

 Lightbulb Institutes Delving Deeper
List of Institutes Researching Naïve physics or folk physics in infants
Things We Don’t Know (1 researchers)

HebiFot (CC0 Public Domain via Pixabay)
Birds have critical periods for learning – but do humans? Image credit: Public domain
The time during which you can learn something is known as a Critial Period, and they are relatively common in the animal kingdom. For example, a newly hatched chicks will imprint on the first suitable moving stimulus they see after hatching, deciding that that thing is its mother, and what it itself looks like. If they don't imprint in the first 14-16 hours, the opportunity has passed, and they will never imprint on anything.

The evidence in humans is limited, but it does seem that for some aspects of language, there is a window after which the opportunity for learning has passed. One of the strongest pieces of evidence comes from the few examples of feral children, raised with very little human interaction. The most studied case was of Genie, who was kept tied to a chair in a silent room for most of the first 13 years of her life. When she and her mother finally escaped, she spoke only a few words, and couldn’t chew or straighten her arms and legs. However, with help, she rapidly begin acquiring new vocabulary, and stringing together pairs of words. It seemed the critical periods theory was in trouble – it was possibly to learn language after early childhood. But, unfortunately, her progress didn’t continue. She remained stuck at this stage, and was never able to grasp grammatical rules or put together whole sentences. So for grammar, at least, this supported the critical periods hypothesis. But cases like this are rare, and it is impossible to say whether her years of abuse were part of the problem.

Now, most scientists talk about sensitive periods rather than critical periods – times when our brains are more ready to absorb new information. This doesn’t mean we can’t do it at other times, but during these sensitive periods learning is easier and faster.
Learn more about whether humans have critical periods for learning.

 Lightbulb Institutes Delving Deeper
List of Institutes Researching whether humans have critical periods for learning
Things We Don’t Know (1 researchers)
2

As they grow up, it becomes a little easier to work out what children have learnt about the world they live in. One ability that takes a while to grasp is understanding how other people think and feel – this is known as Theory of Mind (ToM). Typically, children can pass ToM tests around the age of 4-5. The most common is the Sally-Ann test, which is usually acted out for the child using puppets. The story goes: Sally and Ann are friends. Sally has a marble. She puts in in her box then leaves the room. Ann comes in and moves the marble from the box to the basket. She leaves. Sally comes back in, and wants her marble. Where does she look?

Young children will say she looks in the basket, while older children can understand her false belief – that she thinks it is in the box. But how this understanding develops isn't known. Is it simply that 4-5 years of learning are needed to grasp ToM? Or is there something in the brain that changes to allow this ability to develop? It’s the age old question; nature or nurture. And the answer is that we don’t know for sure – and there is no ethical way to find out, because we can’t keep a child in isolation for 5 years to see if their brain differs from other children's.

Another complicating factor is that looking time tasks have shown infants are surprised when Sally looks in the wrong place (where the marble is rather than where she left it) at less than 2 years of age! So could it be that they know, on some level, where she ought to look, but can't articulate it? It could be that rather than the development of ToM, we're seeing the development of inhibition, and under 4s can't inhibit their own knowledge so as to give the right answer.

Another complication is that the pass age changes depending on the difficulty of the test. At around 2 years, children can understand intentions, and when something is done by accident rather than on purpose. Conversely the ability to recognise a social faux-pas that could cause embarrassment doesn't develop until around the age of 9. Also, young children fail on similar tasks which don't involve a false belief. An example of this would be identifying where an object was when a photo was taken of the scene, when it has been moved since. All this means we are a long way from understanding exactly when and how children develop ToM.

 Lightbulb Institutes Delving Deeper
List of Institutes Researching Theory of Mind in infants
Things We Don’t Know (1 researchers)

Play is thought to be vital for children to learn about the physical and social world around them, but we don't know what it is about playing that is so important. We see play across the animal kingdom in more intelligent and social animals. This suggests it has an evolutionary advantage, but not for simpler animals like fruit flies. Exactly what this advantage is is not known – it could equally be that it is only more intelligent animals that have time to play, or that extended childhoods allow it.
Learn more about why do we play?.

 Lightbulb Institutes Delving Deeper
List of Institutes Researching why do we play?
Things We Don’t Know (1 researchers)
2
naturepost (CC0 Public Domain via Pixabay)
Play is vital for learning in humans and some other animals. Image credit: Public domain

Public Domain via Olichel (Pixabay)
Do computer games help kids learn? Image credit: Public domain
Like them or loathe them, play for many children now includes video games. But what impact is this having on their cognitive and social development? Studies have given conflicting results. One study in Oxford showed that up to an hour of gaming a day actually had some benefits for a child, and it is only when play time exceeds 3 hours that negatives were seen. Scientists think this is because games contain tasks that require problem-solving, memory, and reaction skills, which might help train the learning brain. However, whether this carries over into other aspects of life remains controversial. It seems clear that a moderate amount of gaming won't harm most children – too much of it, however, results in loss of outdoors and social play, which are important for cognitive development.

 Lightbulb

If playing video games can actually help cognitive development, surely games actually designed for the purpose would be even better? If you could improve your memory, attention and reaction time just by playing a few simple games for 15 minutes a day, wouldn’t you want to? Brain training games are marketing this promise – but does it work?

A review published in the journal Psychological Science in the Public Interest suggests the evidence for their benefit is weak[3]. Researchers analysed 374 studies of brain training – all the literature cited by the leading brain training companies. They found that the majority of studies didn’t measure up to the best practice they had defined and weren’t good science. The problems found ranged from small sample sizes to a lack of control group or baseline analysis to compare the post-training results to. Without this, any findings are meaningless. Many studies also failed to account for the placebo effect – if you are told that playing a game will make you better at something, you may get better at it just because you expect to, with no help from the game at all. With all these problems, most of the studies just can’t be relied upon.

There were a few good studies amongst those reviewed, but they didn't show that video game brain training helped improve brain function. Proponents often liken the brain to a muscle, and say that by exercising it, like we do a muscle, we can keep it working well. But this analogy is flawed: if you start lifting weights at the gym, working on your upper body strength, you can rightly expect this exercise to provide benefits in your everyday life – making it easier for you to carry heavy shopping bags or helping you to swim faster. But imagine if that didn’t happen. All the work you put in at the gym meant you were able to lift heavier and heavier dumbbells while you were there, but when you tried to lift something different, or use the muscle in another way, couldn't. This is what seems to be happening with brain training games. People who use them do become better at the specific games they are practising, but they don't see general benefits.

So is there anything you can do to ensure your brain stays healthy? Luckily, the answer is yes – exercise. There is a lot of evidence that physical exercise has benefits for the brain as well as the body, as does a healthy diet and an active social life. So if you want to stave off the declines of ageing, you might want to put down the video games and sign up to that social running club!
Learn more about /aging.

 Lightbulb Institutes Delving Deeper
List of Institutes Researching Brain training games
Things We Don’t Know (2 researchers)
2
© Copyright Albert Bridge
Running is good for your body and brain. Image credit: © Copyright Albert Bridge 

Unsplash (CC0 Public Domain via Pixabay)
Teenagers brains are still developing, which may lead to their mood swings and risky behaviour. Image credit: Public domain
The brain changes a lot throughout childhood. New connections form and old connections are pruned away. But this process doesn't stop once you reach adolescence – some regions, like the cortex, are still undergoing changes into the early 20s. This leads to a strange mix of mature and immature abilities; teens' emotional brain regions are similar to adults (or even more reactive) and the pleasure-seeking parts of their brains are well connected, but their inhibition mechanisms and impulse control are under-developed. This may help explain some of the common teenage behaviour like risk taking and mood swings. Brain scanning studies have also shown that, during certain tasks, teens and adults use different brain regions, possibly demanding different amounts of effort.

But why do some regions of the brain mature later than others?

One theory is that it has an evolutionary benefit, encouraging teenagers to break away from their parents, take risks and explore the world. But we just don't know.

 Lightbulb Institutes Delving Deeper
List of Institutes Researching teenagers' brains
Things We Don’t Know (1 researchers)

Boost your learning

PublicDomainPictures (CC0 Public Domain via Pixabay)
Can music help you learn better? Image credit: Public domain
Many teenagers, and adults, like listening to music while they are working or revising – but does this help or hinder learning? One famous study found that listening to classical music in advance can boost performance on some tasks, "the Mozart effect". More recent studies, however, have shown that this works just as well with other types of music that the listener enjoys, and that the effect is very short-lived. Scientists believe it could be simply increasing arousal, and so helping boost people's alertness, allowing them to concentrate better.

But what about playing music while you work? Studies suggest test performance is generally reduced when music is played (whether or not you like it). More stimulating music seems to be worse than calm relaxing music, but results vary from person to person, with extroverts less effected. Vocals can also limit positive impacts from music, perhaps because the words are distracting. Performance can be boosted with music – if the task is boring and repetitive, but not during learning.

 Lightbulb

Prebiotics provide nutrients for intrinsic gut bacteria and make the metabolism more efficient. Some evidence suggests they can also sharpen your mind, and scientists have suggested that they might impact children’s progress in school, and are looking to test whether this is so.

 Lightbulb Institutes Delving Deeper
List of Institutes Researching Precognitive effects of prebiotics
Things We Don’t Know (1 researchers)
2

Animal Learning

Learning is crucial for surival, and can be seen even in very simple animals like slime mould. These amazing creatures can be acellular: containing multiple nuclei inside one cell, like an amoeba. As they grow, they spread out in all directions before retracting the branches that don't find food and strengthening those that do. This allows them to learn where food is, and even to map the most efficient route through a maze, or recreate the Tokyo subway!

Even more amazingly, slime moulds can learn to anticipate events. In one study, researchers blasted them with cold air at regular intervals, slowing their growth. After a while, they began to slow down just before the time when the next blast of air would be administered. They don't have anything resembling a brain, so nothing in the mould should be able to store a memory, meaning the mystery remains; how do they learn?

 Lightbulb

Machine learning

Can we teach machines to learn?

Researchers have been attempting to teach machines to learn images using artificial neural networks (ANNs), a family of statistical learning models inspired by biological neural networks. A network consists of 10-30 stacked layers of artificial neurones. When an input stimulus, such as an image, is provided, the first layer will communicate with the subsequent layers until the final layer is reached and it generates an output. Knowing how each layer analyses the image differently is challenging, but the idea is that each layer focusses on a progressively higher abstract aspect of the image. To test their results, researchers asked the machines to output images, and found the AI was able to generate a picture of a banana: a weird, fuzzy, banana, but a recognisable one.

Even though these processes are based on well-known mathematical models, we understand very little about why they work, and why certain others don't. This ability to visualise images and produce them from noise has been dubbed “dreaming”.

Scientists are also interested in the role emotions play in learning. Is it different to feel emotions rather than to recognise and understand them? They are now employing learning techniques such as Q-learning and other model-free machine learning processes to probe reward-based learning that mimics human emotions.

In the “mAIry’s Room” thought experiment, mAIry is confined to a grey room from birth and accesses the outside only through a black and white monitor. She is a brilliant scientist and has learnt all there is to learn about colour and perception. One day she experiences purple for the first time. The question asked by philosopher is, has she learnt anything? mAIry doesn't gain any extra explicit knowledge, but her brain has done something new in its processing.

This is known as TD-Lambda (temporal difference) learning: learning by the difference between a predicted reward and the actual reward. Where learning is the goal, AI can treat certain inputs as “special” or an “experience”, valuing them beyond their informational value. These inputs are thus AI “values”. This then allows smart AI to hold evolving values or ideas about what is “right”.
Learn more about /emotions.

 Lightbulb Institutes Delving Deeper
List of Institutes Researching AI learning
Things We Don’t Know (1 researchers)
3
Google Research Blog
Google's new AI can output this image of 'bananas' Image credit: Google Research Blog 

GabboT via Wikipedia Commons.
How is machine learning like human learning? Image credit: GabboT via Wikipedia Commons. 
Scientists can also bypass our understanding of the learning process and teach machines to learn directly by hooking them up to our brain. New brain-computer-interface (BCI) technology can now be implanted into the motor cortex of the brain and calibrated in ~37s by people with tetraplegia/quadriplegia to let them move cursors and control robotic arms that type for them, achieving a typing rate of 39 characters per minute or more. This technology relies upon advances in statistical learning algorithms that allow the BCI system to rapidly learn the neural signals that link to various motor intentions, and could soon be clinically available.

 Lightbulb Institutes Delving Deeper
List of Institutes Researching Machine Learning
Things We Don’t Know (1 researchers)

Machine learning can be used in all kinds of fields. For example, new predictive models have been created using machine learning and random forest classification to identify how proteins bind to engineered nanomaterials in biological and environmental systems[4]. This work could have implications in pollution remediation and drug delivery.

 Lightbulb Institutes Delving Deeper
List of Institutes Researching Machine learning for protein-coated nanoparticles
Things We Don’t Know (1 researchers)
3

Another research area that may benefit from machine learning is making organic crystals. Although researchers now have banks and banks of data on what will and what will not crystallise, there is no simple relationship, and a huge processing demand to try to unravel it. However, researchers at Oxford University including Richard Cooper are currently investigating the use of AI to better predict which crystals will and will not form from organic molecules. AI employs machine learning tactics to look for statistical patterns across large data sets and make predictions that expand on the set. So far, results have looked promising.
Learn more about /artificial intelligence.

 Lightbulb Institutes Delving Deeper
List of Institutes Researching AIs and Organic Solids
Things We Don’t Know (1 researchers)

This article was written by the Things We Don’t Know editorial team, with contributions from Ed Trollope, Ginny Smith, Cait Percy, Rowena Fletcher-Wood, and Holly Godwin.

This article was first published on and was last updated on 2021-07-23.

References
why don’t all references have links?

[1] Vogel, G., (2012). Can We Make Our Brains More Plastic? Science 338(6103):36-39. doi: 10.1126/science.338.6103.36.
[2] Miller, G., (2012). How Are Memories Retrieved? Science 338(6103):30-31. doi: 10.1126/science.338.6103.30-b.
[3] Simons, D,J., et al., (2016). Do Brain-Training Programs Work? Psychological Science in the Public Interest 17(3):103-186. doi: 10.1177/1529100616661983.
[4] Findlay, Matthew R., et al. Machine learning provides predictive analysis into silver nanoparticle protein corona formation from physicochemical properties. Environmental Science: Nano 5.1 (2018): 64-71.

Recent learning News

Get customised news updates on your homepage by subscribing to articles

atom icon
AI thought X-rays are connected to eating refried beans or drinking beer
13th December, 2024
Instead of finding true medical insights, these algorithms sometimes rely on irrelevant factors — leading to misleading results.
atom icon
AI is scheming to stay online — and then lying to humans
13th December, 2024
An alarming third party report almost looks like a prequel to Terminator.
atom icon
Google’s DeepMind claims they have the best weather prediction model
5th December, 2024
After chess, Go, and proteins, has DeepMind taken over weather forecasting as well?
atom icon
Main Sequence and White Dwarf Binaries are Hiding in Plain Sight
20th November, 2024
Common envelope binaries are the progenitors of Type1a supernovae, but little is known about this phase. A new catalogue will help.
atom icon
This smart sensor can detect health symptoms without cloud computing
30th October, 2024
Sensor patches could transform healthcare and health monitoring.


Follow TWDK

Mailing list

Sign up for our Newsletter
constant contact safe subscribe logo

Fundraising

Easyfundraising banner

Creative Commons License
Except where otherwise noted, content on this site by Things We Don’t Know C.I.C. is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike 4.0 International License. | Privacy & Cookies
Things We Don’t Know C.I.C. is registered in England and Wales. Company Number 8109669.
Registered address at 34B York Way, London, N1 9AB.